Inflating and deflating the self: Sustaining motivational concerns through self-evaluation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.11.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Self-evaluations may shift dynamically to serve motivational concerns.

  • Self-evaluation inflation is associated with promotion focus concerns.

  • Self-evaluation deflation is associated with prevention focus concerns.

  • Results support the idea of strategic shifts in self-evaluation positivity.

Abstract

The ways in which individuals think and feel about themselves play a significant role in guiding behavior across many domains in life. The current studies investigate how individuals may shift the positivity of self-evaluations in order to sustain their chronic or momentary motivational concerns. Specifically, we propose that more positive self-evaluations support eagerness that sustains promotion-focused concerns with advancement, whereas less positive self-evaluations support vigilance that sustains prevention-focused concerns with safety. The current studies provide evidence that self-evaluation inflation is associated with promotion concerns whereas self-evaluation deflation is associated with prevention concerns, whether regulatory focus is situationally manipulated (Studies 1, 2b, and 3) or measured as a chronic individual difference (Study 2a). Following regulatory focus primes, individuals in a promotion focus showed relatively greater accessibility of positive versus negative self-knowledge compared to individuals in a prevention focus (Study 1). In an ongoing performance situation, participants in a promotion focus reported higher self-esteem than participants in a prevention focus (Studies 2a and 2b). Finally, individuals in a promotion focus persisted longer on an anagram task when given an opportunity to focus on their strengths versus weaknesses, which was not the case for individuals in a prevention focus (Study 3). Across studies, the predicted interactions were consistently obtained, although sometimes the effects were stronger for promotion or prevention motivation. We discuss implications for existing models of the motives underlying self-evaluation.

Introduction

Life provides ample opportunities for boosts and blows to our sense of self, occasions that often prompt self-reflection and assessment. These self-musings cut across domains, influencing behavior in almost every life arena, and can be guided by a number of underlying motivational concerns. Often we just want to feel good about ourselves; we find ways to protect and even enhance our self-views (e.g., Hepper, Gramzow, & Sedikides, 2010). Sometimes we want confirmation that others see us the way we see ourselves, whether that view is positive or negative (e.g., Swann, Rentfrow, & Guinn, 2003). Sometimes we seek accuracy and certainty, even if it does not enhance or verify (e.g., Trope, 1986, Trope and Brickman, 1975). Sometimes we want to know how we can do better in the future (e.g., Taylor et al., 1995, Wood, 1989). In general, research in self-evaluation has focused on these four motives underlying self-evaluation—self-enhancement, self-verification, self-assessment, and self-improvement (Gregg et al., 2011, Sedikides and Strube, 1995, Sedikides and Strube, 1997). In this paper, we explore an additional motive underlying self-evaluation—to sustain underlying motivational concerns. Specifically, we propose that individuals' self-evaluations can sustain or disrupt their underlying motivational concerns and that, depending on these concerns, individuals may be differentially motivated to enhance or deflate the positivity of self-views.

There are multiple motives for self-evaluation. Understanding the relation among these motives and the circumstances under which particular motives will dominate has been an aim of self-evaluation research (Chang-Schneider and Swann, 2010, Hepper et al., 2010, Sedikides and Strube, 1995, Stinson et al., 2010, Swann, 1984, Swann and Schroeder, 1995, Taylor et al., 1995, Trope, 1986). In general, motives regarding self-protection (e.g., avoiding negative self-views) and self-enhancement (e.g., promoting more positive self-views) have been seen as fundamental (Alicke and Sedikides, 2009, Brown, 2010, Hepper et al., 2010, Leary and Baumeister, 2000, Sedikides and Strube, 1995, Steele, 1988, Tesser, 2001, Yamaguchi et al., 2007). There is considerable support for the notion that the motive to protect and enhance a positive self-view is primary, though cultures may differ in the ways in which this motive is expressed (e.g., Chiu, Wan, Cheng, Kim, & Yang, 2011). In general, however, people see more good than bad when they look in the mirror (Alicke, 1985), see their own traits as particularly desirable (Dunning, Leuenberger, & Sherman, 1995), and believe that their future is blessed relative to others (Weinstein, 1980). When cognitive resources are constrained, the self-enhancement motive appears to be the default (Hixon and Swann, 1993, Swann et al., 1990, Swann and Schroeder, 1995).

When, then, are individuals motivated to see the darker or weaker sides of themselves? Research suggests that people will evaluate themselves less positively or seek potentially negative self-information primarily in circumstances when motives for self-verification, accurate self-assessment, or self-improvement are activated. For pragmatic reasons, individuals may prefer a relationship partner who contributes to their self-verification, facilitating smooth interpersonal interaction. For instance, although people prefer their dating partners to see them positively (regardless of how they see themselves), people prefer their married partners to see them as they see themselves (self-verification) (Swann, De La Ronde, & Hixon, 1994). Epistemic concerns may also support negative self-evaluation; confirming one's views of self, even if negative, can provide a sense of coherence and stability (Swann, Stein-Seroussi, & Giesler, 1992). When traits are seen as important and modifiable, individuals may be willing to see weakness if it corresponds to accurate self-assessment or facilitates self-improvement (Dunning, 1995, Trope, 1986). This type of research suggests that negative self-views are functional to the extent that they support objective or subjective truths about the self.

However, some evidence suggests that negative self-evaluations might have an additional self-regulatory function beyond their conveyance of some (oft painful) truth. For example, Baumeister, Tice, and Hutton (1989) proposed that expressed self-evaluations might be strategically employed to deflect or create expectations in others; self-evaluation may be self-presentational. Indicating low self-esteem or low confidence in oneself can be one way to manage the expectations of others in the face of possible failure (e.g., “I'm not so hot. Don't count on me to save the day.”). Dampened self-evaluations may be embraced not only in an attempt to have a balanced or accurate view of oneself, but also because they can be strategically useful in interpersonal interactions. Embracing a dampened view of the self is a cautious tactic that can minimize regret if things go awry (cf. Josephs, Larrick, Steele, & Nisbett, 1992).

Research on defensive pessimism further suggests that negative self-views can be helpful not only to manage the expectations of others, but also to increase one's own motivation and engagement in goal pursuit (Norem, 2008, Norem and Cantor, 1986a, Norem and Cantor, 1986b, Showers, 1992). Defensive pessimists “expect the worst” when entering a new situation, despite the fact that they generally do not perform differently than those with a more optimistic outlook (Cantor and Norem, 1989, Cantor et al., 1987). In contrast to “true” pessimism, defensive pessimism serves two goals—a self-protective goal of preparation for possible failure in the future and a motivational goal of increasing vigilance in the present to prevent potential negative outcomes in the future (Seery, West, Weisbuch, & Blascovich, 2008). In support of this idea, it has been shown that when the strategic coping mechanisms of defensive pessimists were disrupted, they performed more poorly (Norem and Cantor, 1986b, Showers, 1992). Simply pointing out the inconsistency between their current expectations and past performance disrupted their ability to harness vigilance in their preferred way (Norem & Cantor, 1986b). Furthermore, defensive pessimists who focused on negative rather than positive outcome possibilities for an upcoming social interaction talked more, exerted more effort, and had their conversations rated more positively by the confederates with whom they were interacting (Showers, 1992). This research suggests that for some individuals, focusing on negativity may be used to manage the self more effectively.

We propose that the dynamics observed in defensive pessimism (focusing on and harnessing negativity as a way to self-regulate) may be one example of a broader motivation underlying self-evaluation. Specifically, individuals may strategically shift their self-views not only downwards, but also upwards, as a way to sustain and maintain preferred motivational orientations. Our perspective suggests a dynamic, pro-active view of self-evaluation; self-evaluations are not always retrospective reflections on the past, but may be strategically shifted to serve future self-regulation and goal pursuit. Furthermore, because individuals may differ in the extent to which inflated versus deflated self-evaluation sustains or disrupts their chronic or momentary motivational concerns, individuals may be differentially motivated to inflate or deflate the self. Individuals may therefore shift the positivity of self-evaluations to support the strategic orientation that best sustains or “fits” their motivational concerns (e.g., Higgins, 2000).

For example, for individuals who need to be eager to sustain their motivational concerns, more positive self-evaluations should support the eagerness they need. In contrast, for individuals who need to be vigilant to sustain their motivational concerns, relatively less positive self-evaluations should support the vigilance they need. Because individuals differ in whether they have motivational concerns that are sustained by either eagerness or vigilance, self-evaluations could be enhanced or dampened tactically in the service of supporting what people need motivationally. This should be true based on an individual's current motivational state, both when that state arises from chronic individual differences and when it arises temporarily from situational induction. We examined these ideas by taking advantage of past research on regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997). In the next section, we outline the connections between regulatory focus theory and the current predictions.

Regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997) identifies two underlying self-regulatory systems (promotion, prevention) that have been differentially associated with the use of eager versus vigilant strategies (Crowe and Higgins, 1997, Grant and Higgins, 2003, Higgins and Molden, 2003, Liberman et al., 2001, Molden and Higgins, 2005, Scholer and Higgins, 2008). Individuals can differ both chronically and temporarily in their sensitivity to promotion versus prevention concerns (e.g., Higgins et al., 2001, Higgins et al., 1994, Higgins et al., 1997), which in turn influence strategic preferences.

The promotion system is concerned with hopes and aspirations (ideals) and accomplishments. Individuals in a promotion focus are sensitive to gains versus nongains (i.e., the difference between “0” and “+1”) and they regulate more effectively using eager approach strategies (Higgins, 2000, Higgins, 2005, Higgins, 2009). Eager strategies serve promotion concerns because they are about enthusiastically pursuing potential gain in the pursuit of advancement and growth. Indeed, individuals in a promotion focus are more motivated by positive role models (Lockwood et al., 2002, Schokker et al., 2010) and perform better after receiving success feedback that increases eagerness (Idson & Higgins, 2000; see also Idson et al., 2004, Van-Dijk and Kluger, 2004).

In contrast, the prevention system is concerned with duties and responsibilities (oughts) and security. Individuals in a prevention focus are sensitive to losses versus nonlosses (i.e., the difference between “0” and “− 1”) and they regulate more effectively using vigilant avoidance strategies (Higgins, 2000, Higgins, 2005, Higgins, 2009). Vigilant strategies serve prevention concerns because they are about carefully avoiding potential loss and guarding against mistakes in order to ensure safety and maintain a satisfactory state (see also Brodscholl, Kober, & Higgins, 2007). Indeed, individuals in a prevention focus are more motivated by negative role models (Lockwood et al., 2002, Schokker et al., 2010) and perform better after receiving failure feedback that increases vigilance (Idson and Higgins, 2000, Idson et al., 2004, Van-Dijk and Kluger, 2004).

Given that individuals in a promotion versus prevention focus regulate more effectively using eager versus vigilant strategies, respectively, individuals may differ in their need for positive self-evaluation depending on the motivational concern that is most salient in a given situation. Positive or enhanced self-evaluations highlight strengths and anticipated successes, which support the eagerness that sustains promotion concerns. Less positive or dampened self-evaluations highlight weaknesses and potential failures, which support the vigilance that sustains prevention concerns.

If one of the functions of self-evaluation is to help regulate strategic orientations that fit motivational concerns (e.g., eagerness for promotion concerns; vigilance for prevention concerns), then individuals may be motivated to increase or decrease self-evaluations in ways that support the strategic orientation that fits their concerns (cf. Förster, Grant, Idson, & Higgins, 2001). In particular, in order to support the strategic eagerness that sustains or fits promotion concerns, individuals with a promotion focus (whether from chronic predisposition or situationally induced) should be more likely to inflate positive self-evaluations, such as strongly endorsing positive items on a self-esteem questionnaire or emphasizing strengths instead of weaknesses. In contrast, in order to support the strategic vigilance that sustains or fits prevention concerns, individuals with a prevention focus (again whether from chronic predisposition or situationally induced) should be more likely to deflate or rein in the positivity of their self-evaluations, such as endorsing less positive or negative items on a self-esteem questionnaire or emphasizing weaknesses instead of strengths.

In addition to suggesting a new motivation underlying the self-evaluation process (i.e., self-evaluation as a tactic to sustain individuals' motivational concerns), these predictions also expand existing research relating regulatory focus and self-evaluation. Prior research has investigated the relation between regulatory focus and self-evaluation in terms of preferences for self-protection strategies versus self-enhancement strategies (Hepper et al., 2010) and goals for self-enhancement versus self-certainty (Leonardelli and Lakin, 2010, Leonardelli et al., 2007). Hepper et al. (2010) found that promotion-focused individuals, who are more sensitive to gains and nongains, are more likely to report using self-enhancement strategies (e.g., “when you do poorly at something, reminding yourself of your strengths and abilities”), whereas prevention-focused individuals, who are more sensitive to losses and nonlosses, are more likely to report using self-protection strategies (e.g., “associating yourself with people who are successful, but not more successful than you”). Other research (Leonardelli et al., 2007) has shown that individuals in a promotion focus have more highly accessible self-enhancement goals, whereas individuals in a prevention focus have more highly accessible self-certainty goals. Achieving clarity and certainty about one's self-views may be important to individuals in a prevention focus as a way to achieve a sense of security.

The findings of these research programs are generally consistent with our proposal, to the extent that promotion motivation is associated more with eager-related self-evaluation processes and prevention motivation is associated more with vigilant-related self-evaluation processes. However, although previous research has linked the promotion system to preferences for inflated self-evaluation, this research has not investigated links between the prevention system and deflated self-evaluations. In addition, previous research has not examined how inflated versus deflated self-evaluations relate to sustaining promotion versus prevention motivations, respectively. Prior research has not investigated the idea that self-evaluation may be motivated by the desire to sustain a preferred motivational orientation—both when that motivational orientation is chronic and when it is situationally induced. These questions are addressed in the present research.

We present four studies that explore the possibility that individuals may be motivated to enhance or deflate positive views of the self in the service of sustaining their chronic or situationally induced motivational concerns. In Study 1, we first tested the idea that individuals in a promotion versus prevention focus should differentially activate positive versus negative aspects of self-knowledge. Activation of positive self-knowledge indicates potential success, supporting eagerness, whereas activation of negative self-knowledge indicates potential failure, supporting vigilance. In Studies 2 and 3, we directly tested the prediction that individuals in a promotion versus prevention focus should be differentially motivated to protect and enhance the self in ongoing, but not completed, performance situations. Study 4 investigated how engaging in self-inflating or self-deflating activities directly affects the motivational persistence of individuals in a promotion versus prevention focus, respectively.

Section snippets

Study 1

Study 1 examined whether self-knowledge would be selectively activated according to its relevance for motivational concerns. An individual possesses a wide range of self-knowledge, but only a small subset of knowledge is activated at any one time (Markus & Wurf, 1987). Motivation is known to be one of the factors to influence what knowledge becomes accessible, such that the subset of self-knowledge that is most relevant to successful attainment of a current goal is most likely to be activated (

Participants

Participants were 29 undergraduate students (15 men, 13 women and 1 unknown) at a national university in Tokyo, Japan.1 They agreed to participate in a two-session experiment, 40 min per

Latency

Four participants were excluded from the response latency analysis because their responses to positive or negative traits in session 1 and/or session 2 were all no. This procedure left 25 participants for the further analysis. We also examined the data for possible outliers and, following convention (cf. Ferguson, 2008), excluded reaction times (7 responses) that were slower by more than 3 SDs from each individual's mean.2

Studies 2a and 2b

Study 1 provided some initial support for the idea that the activation of the promotion and prevention systems may influence the accessibility of positive and negative self-knowledge, such that prevention system concerns may be linked to a relatively deflated (less positive) self-view, whereas promotion system concerns may be linked to a relatively inflated (more positive) self-view (as reflected in the results on the relative accessibility index). If these shifts serve a strategic

Participants

Participants were134 undergraduates at a private university in the northeastern United States (85 females, 49 males) who completed a 45-minute session for credit or payment ($8). Participants were randomly assigned to condition in a 2 (performance status: ongoing, completed) × 2 (feedback: success, failure) between-participants design. Chronic regulatory focus was assessed using the regulatory focus questionnaire; RFQ (Higgins et al., 2001). In a funnel debriefing (cf. Chartrand & Bargh, 1996),

Analysis overview

The data were analyzed using a continuous regulatory focus predominance measure (promotion pride  prevention pride), as has been done in other regulatory focus research (e.g., Bohns et al., 2013, Cesario et al., 2004, Hong and Lee, 2008). Status and feedback were effect-coded. Rosenberg self-esteem was computed by summing the responses to the 10 items, such that scores could range from 7 to 70.

Mood manipulation check

As expected, participants who received negative performance feedback reported feeling worse (M = 5.18, SD =

Study 2b

Study 2b was conducted to provide a conceptual replication of Study 2a with manipulated rather than measured regulatory focus and with a non-undergraduate student population. In addition, because the feedback manipulation did not interact with regulatory focus in Study 2a, it was not included in this study. The Rosenberg (1965) measure of self-esteem again served as the assessment of self-evaluation (Cronbach's α = .94).

Participants

Participants were 113 individuals (67 females, 2 transgender, Mage = 31.29 years, SD = 12.57) who completed an online study for monetary compensation through the Amazon Mechanical Turk website (see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). 16 participants did not complete all items on the Rosenberg self-esteem scale and were excluded from the analyses. The regulatory focus × performance status interaction on self-esteem remains statistically significant with the full sample.8

Results and discussion

There was no main effect of performance status, F(1, 93) = .05, p = .82, partial η2 = .001, nor regulatory focus, F(1, 93) = .78, p = .38, partial η2 = .008, on self-esteem. As predicted, however, there was a significant performance status × regulatory focus interaction on self-esteem, F(1, 93) = 5.69, p = .019, partial η2 = .06 (see Fig. 3). As in Study 2a, there was no significant difference in self-esteem between promotion and prevention-focused participants in the completed condition, F(1, 52) = 1.58, p = .21,

Study 3

Studies 2a and 2b provided evidence that in an ongoing performance situation, individuals may be motivated to maintain a self-evaluation that supports their strategic orientation. In an ongoing performance situation, individuals in a promotion focus reported more positive self-evaluations relative to individuals in a prevention focus. However, these studies did not directly test whether self-evaluation inflation and deflation has an impact on subsequent motivation. Will individuals in a

Participants

Participants were 98 undergraduate students (57 females) at a small liberal arts college in the United States who completed the study for partial course credit or payment. Participants were randomly assigned to condition in a 2 (regulatory focus: prevention, promotion) × 2 (anagram practice type: self-inflating, self-deflating) design.

Procedure

Participants were run in individual sessions. Participants were told that they would be completing a number of brief, unrelated studies associated generally with

Manipulation Check

To check whether type of anagram practice affected state eagerness and vigilance, we conducted a 2 (regulatory focus) × 2 (practice type) ANOVA on the eagerness/vigilance index. There was a main effect of practice type, F(1,94) = 10.82, p = .001, partial η2 = .10, such that participants in the self-deflating condition were relatively more vigilant (M =  .16, SD = 1.99, CI =  .71, .39) than participants in the self-boosting condition (M = 1.15, SD = 1.91, CI = .58, 1.71). There was no main effect of regulatory

Discussion

Study 3 provides further support for the idea that self-inflation and self-deflation may differentially motivate individuals depending on their motivational state. An analysis comparing fit (promotion–self-inflation; prevention–self-deflation) versus nonfit (promotion–self-deflation; prevention–self-inflation) conditions found the predicted effect. However, the predicted contrasts were statistically significant only for participants in the promotion condition. When individuals in a promotion

General discussion

The current studies provide initial support for the idea that self-evaluation may serve a self-regulatory function of helping individuals maintain the strategic motivational state that fits or sustains their current goal pursuit orientation. In particular, individuals may inflate self-evaluation in order to support eagerness but deflate self-evaluation in order to support vigilance. Individuals in a promotion focus, who regulate more effectively with eager strategies, showed greater

References (103)

  • C. Steele

    The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self

  • W.B. Swann et al.

    Self-verification processes: How we sustain our self-conceptions

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (1981)
  • M.D. Alicke

    Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1985)
  • M.D. Alicke et al.

    Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and what they do

    European Review of Social Psychology

    (2009)
  • T. Aoki

    Seikaku hyougen yougo no shinri-jiten teki kenkyu: 455-go no sentaku, bunrui, oyobi nozomashisa no hyoutei [A psycho-lexical study of personality trait words: Selection, classification and desirability ratings of 455 words]

    Japanese Journal of Psychology

    (1971)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Exploding the self-esteem myth

    Scientific American

    (2005)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Self-presentational motivations and personality differences in self-esteem

    Journal of Personality

    (1989)
  • V.K. Bohns et al.

    Opposites fit: Regulatory focus complementarity and relationship well-being

    Social Cognition

    (2013)
  • J.C. Brodscholl et al.

    Strategies of self-regulation in goal attainment versus goal maintenance

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2007)
  • J.D. Brown

    Across the (not so) great divide: Cultural similarities in self-evaluative processes

    Social and Personality Psychology Compass

    (2010)
  • M. Buhrmester et al.

    Amazon's mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive yet high-quality, data?

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • California Task Force to Promote Self-Esteem and Personal and Social Responsibility

    Toward a state of self-esteem

    (1990)
  • N. Cantor et al.

    Defensive pessimism and stress and coping

    Social Cognition

    (1989)
  • N. Cantor et al.

    Life tasks, self-concept ideals, and cognitive strategies in a life transition

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1987)
  • J. Cesario et al.

    Regulatory fit and persuasion: Transfer from “feeling right”

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • C. Chang-Schneider et al.

    The role of uncertainty in self-evaluative processes: Another look at the cognitive-affective crossfire

  • T.L. Chartrand et al.

    Automatic activation of social information processing goals: Nonconscious priming reproduces effects of explicit conscious instructions

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • C. Chiu et al.

    Cultural perspectives on self-enhancement and self-protection

  • E. Diener

    Subjective well-being

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1984)
  • D. Dunning

    Trait importance and modifiability as factors influencing self-assessment and self-enhancement motives

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1995)
  • D. Dunning et al.

    A new look at motivated inference: Are self-serving theories of success a product of motivational forces?

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1995)
  • M.J. Ferguson

    On becoming ready to pursue a goal you don't know you have: Effects of nonconscious goals on evaluative readiness

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2008)
  • J. Förster et al.

    Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus and the “goal looms larger” effect

    Journal of Personality & Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • A.L. Freitas et al.

    Enjoying goal-directed action: The role of regulatory fit

    Psychological Science

    (2002)
  • H. Grant et al.

    Optimism, promotion pride, and prevention pride as predictors of quality of life

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2003)
  • A.P. Gregg et al.

    Quantifying self‐motives: Functional links between dispositional desires

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • M.S. Hagger et al.

    Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta- analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2010)
  • K.L. Haws et al.

    An assessment of chronic regulatory focus measures

    Journal of Marketing Research

    (2010)
  • T.F. Heatherton et al.

    Development and validation of a scale measuring state self-esteem

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1991)
  • S.J. Heine et al.

    Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?

    Psychological Review

    (1999)
  • E.G. Hepper et al.

    Individual differences in self-enhancement and self-protection strategies: An integrative analysis

    Journal of Personality

    (2010)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience

  • E.T. Higgins

    Beyond pleasure and pain

    American Psychologist

    (1997)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Making a good decision: Value from fit

    American Psychologist

    (2000)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Value from regulatory fit

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2005)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Value from hedonic experience and engagement

    Psychological Review

    (2006)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Culture and personality: Variability across universal motives as the missing link

    Social and Personality Psychology Compass

    (2008)
  • E.T. Higgins

    Regulatory fit in the goal-pursuit process

  • E.T. Higgins et al.

    Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2001)
  • E.T. Higgins et al.

    Transfer of value from fit

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2003)
  • Cited by (39)

    • Regulatory focus and self-licensing dynamics: A motivational account of behavioural consistency and balancing

      2022, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      On the one hand, regulatory fit theory postulates that promotion-oriented individuals are more sensitive to positive information and increase their efforts following this type of information, in comparison to negative information (Higgins, 2009; Idson et al., 2000, 2004; Idson & Higgins, 2000; Shu & Lam, 2011, 2016; Van Dijk & Kluger, 2004, 2011) – yet, these studies focused on performance and persistence in different sets of cognitive tasks, but did not address intentions or behaviours related to a moral or higher-value domain. On the other hand, the regulatory closure hypothesis states that both success and failure in a promotion-framed goal would have an activating effect on the individual (the first because it elicits cheerfulness and elation and a sense of progress towards the higher-level goal, and the second because it elicits frustration and anger, and a sense of goal incompletion) and lead to similarly high commitment towards the goal (Ahn et al., 2020; Baas et al., 2011; Lalot et al., 2021; Scholer et al., 2014). The present results mostly support the regulatory closure hypothesis as no significant differences between conditions of feedback arose in a promotion focus in Studies 2–3.

    • Metamotivation: Emerging research on the regulation of motivational states

      2020, Advances in Motivation Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similar to the college students in our original studies (Scholer & Miele, 2016), children reported that they would perform better on the eager vs. vigilant task when promotion-focused, but would perform better on the vigilant vs. eager task when prevention-focused (task knowledge). Children were also asked to report how particular strategies—focusing on their strengths or focusing on their weaknesses—would affect performance, given prior work linking these strategies to the upregulation of promotion and prevention motivation, respectively (Scholer, Ozaki, & Higgins, 2014). Although children exhibited accurate task knowledge, children did not exhibit accurate strategy knowledge with these specific strategies.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported in part by Grant 39429 from the National Institute of Mental Health to E. Tory Higgins and a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Insight Grant to Abigail A. Scholer.

    View full text